Science
This copy is for your personal, non-commercial use only.
Permission to republish or repurpose articles or portions of articles can be obtained by following the guidelines here. The following resources related to this article are available online at www.sciencemag.org (thisinfomation is current as of February 29, 2012 ): Updated information and services, including high-resolution figures, can be found in the online version of this article at: http://www.sciencemag.org/content/312/5776/1038.full.html Supporting Online Material can be found at: http://www.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2006/05/17/312.5776.1038.DC1.html A list of selected additional articles on the ScienceWeb sites related to this article can be found at: http://www.sciencemag.org/content/312/5776/1038.full.html#related This article has been cited by 70 article(s) on the ISI Web of Science This article has been cited by 15 articles hosted by HighWire Press; see: http://www.sciencemag.org/content/312/5776/1038.full.html#related-urls This article appears in the following subject collections: Psychologyhttp://www.sciencemag.org/cgi/collection/psychology
Science (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week in December, by the American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. Copyright 2006 by the American Association for the Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title Science is aregistered trademark of AAAS.
Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on February 29, 2012
If you wish to distribute this article to others, you can order high-quality copies for your colleagues, clients, or customers by clicking here.
REPORTS
Apes Save Tools for Future Use
Nicholas J. Mulcahy and Josep Call* Planning for future needs, not just current ones, is one of the most formidable humancognitive achievements. Whether this skill is a uniquely human adaptation is a controversial issue. In a study we conducted, bonobos and orangutans selected, transported, and saved appropriate tools above baseline levels to use them 1 hour later (experiment 1). Experiment 2 extended these results to a 14-hour delay between collecting and using the tools. Experiment 3 showed that seeing the apparatusduring tool selection was not necessary to succeed. These findings suggest that the precursor skills for planning for the future evolved in great apes before 14 million years ago, when all extant great ape species shared a common ancestor. ulving (1) recounts an Estonian tale of a girl who dreamed about attending a party but was unable to eat her favorite dessert because there were no spoonsavailable. Facing the possibility of attending the party again, she took a spoon to bed. Crucially, the girl took the spoon not because she currently needed it, but because she would need it in the future. Tulving used this example to illustrate the putatively unique human ability to think about the past and plan for the future (2–4) and proposed that an analogous Bspoon[ test could be used to test forfuture planning in nonhuman animals. Future planning is cognitively demanding because it imposes a long delay between performing an action and getting rewarded for it: a skill that humans use when preparing a suitcase before a trip or by making a cake to celebrate someone_s birthday. Although various animals can plan and execute multiple actions toward a goal (5, 6), they may achieve this withouttaking into account future needs, just current ones (3, 4, 7). Thus, when chimpanzees transport stones to use them to crack open nuts, or New Caledonian crows make hook-shaped tools to fish for insects, they do so in an attempt to satisfy their current hunger state, not some future one.
T
Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany. *To...
Regístrate para leer el documento completo.